ADVERTISEMENT
  About the SA Blog Network













Observations

Observations


Opinion, arguments & analyses from the editors of Scientific American
Observations HomeAboutContact

Tumors and concealed weapons beware, ophthalmology is teaching us how to become better searchers

The views expressed are those of the author and are not necessarily those of Scientific American.


Email   PrintPrint



searching rare items luggage tumorsHunting for a misplaced set of keys or a dead cell phone can be a nuisance. But for people who search for concealed weapons or malignant tumors, finding a target—and one they’re not sure is even there—could be a matter of life or death. Unfortunately, research has shown that the rarer an item has proved to be, the less likely people are to find it when it is there.

"We know that if you don’t find it often, you often don’t find it," Jeremy Wolfe, a professor of ophthalmology at Harvard Medical School, said in a prepared statement. Likewise, searches for common objects tend to turn up way more false positives. So are people as hasty to judge that an inspected bag doesn’t contain a weapon (a rare item), as they are to assume it has a more common item?

Wolfe and his colleague, Michael Van Wert of the Brigham and Women’s Hospital in Cambridge, were determined to hunt down an answer to how reaction times changed based on what people were used to finding. Their results were published online January 14 in Current Biology

Their experiment, in which two dozen participants looked for weapons in simulated baggage, showed that people do not adapt their searching time equally to different odds of finding items. Individuals were given a consistent likelihood of finding an object, say 98 percent, and were judged both on the time it took them to declare whether a bag contained a weapon—and whether they were correct. Those in experiments who are faced with consistently 50-50 odds of finding a target might be expected to take the longest to arrive at an answer, and those with very rare or very frequent incidences of finding weapons should be the fastest. That is, with similarly slim odds of finding or not finding a target, people might be expected to make decisions rapidly about whether they see it or not.

But that’s not what Wolfe and Van Wert’s data revealed. Those who looked for items that were frequently present were quick to ID them (often creating false-positives when the items were not there), but those who had infrequent targets were much slower to dismiss a baggage sample as not containing a target. "When nothing is there, they don’t give up on the response," Wolfe said. If people are bad at finding rare objects, why do we spend so long looking for them anyway?

"It’s all terribly adaptive behavior for a beast in the world," Wolfe said. "If you know berries are there, you keep looking until you find them. If they are never there, you don’t spend your time hunting."

But these ancient adaptations aren’t always so beneficial today, especially for those who are charged with hunting for these rare—and sometimes dangerous—objects. "We aren’t well-built for that and make more errors than we’d like," Wolfe said.

And looking for an unknown number of targets presents an even bigger dilemma. Searchers have to continue looking until they are satisfied with their hunt. This "’satisfaction of search’ is a known problem in search for an unknown number of targets," the authors wrote. "The probability of detecting one target is lower if another target has been detected first."

All of this knowledge, however, could help those who search for objects, such as airport security personnel or medical technicians, to do a better job. Wolfe noted that pre-shift training in which a person was exposed to high frequency examples before they went on duty might help them find that rare hidden bomb or malignancy. Though it might not help you track down that pen you just misplaced.

Image courtesy of iStockphoto/Yakobchuk





Rights & Permissions

Comments 2 Comments

Add Comment
  1. 1. Michael Hanlon 5:27 am 01/15/2010

    When last we heard from Harvard Medical, they were setting up their radiate the squirrel monkees at Maclean Hospital. Now, they have an ophthalmologist making judgements on neurological issues? The working of the brain does indeed change with training (they need to study why we have schools?). Nothing new there. In the recognition process the brain uses the eyes as a tool to provide data for determinations. What we have with an eye dox making judgements on the workings of the mind is akin to a carpenter deciding whether a twentieth story can be built on a building, Just who is running Harvard medical anyway? Patch Adams?

    Link to this
  2. 2. Michael Hanlon 12:53 am 01/17/2010

    Here’s an idea to make the whole issue moot.
    .Since a person is taveling by a communal method, some personal rights need be given up for the safety of the whole. Just open the damn bags and look inside!

    Link to this

Add a Comment
You must sign in or register as a ScientificAmerican.com member to submit a comment.

More from Scientific American

Scientific American Back To School

Back to School Sale!

12 Digital Issues + 4 Years of Archive Access just $19.99

Order Now >

X

Email this Article



This function is currently unavailable

X