Skip to main content

When Did Insects Learn to Smell?

Summer beckons in the northern latitudes. As temperatures warm, beachgoers will flood sandy coasts, carrying bags of potato chips and sandwiches.

This article was published in Scientific American’s former blog network and reflects the views of the author, not necessarily those of Scientific American


Summer beckons in the northern latitudes. As temperatures warm, beachgoers will flood sandy coasts, carrying bags of potato chips and sandwiches. Picnics will sprout in parks supplied with pink watermelon wedges and soda pop. Feasts ripe for people, and undoubtedly bugs too. In mere seconds flies and bees can swarm, but how do they sniff out food so fast? Knowing how to fly could be the reason, according to new research from the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology.

Their findings challenge an old theory on when insects first evolved olfaction. Bugs share an ancestor with crustaceans, which lack the physical machinery typically tied to a sense of smell. Scientists presumed that when this oceanic predecessor moved ashore approximately 480 million years ago it developed the olfactory hardware for sifting aromas that permeated through its new air-based environment.

“Not true. They kept the old way of smelling for a long time after,” says evolutionary neuroethologist Bill Hansson, who led the recent study published in eLife. To draw this conclusion, Hansson’s team examined two of the most primal insects left on the planet: bristletails (Lepismachilis y-signata) and firebrats (Thermobia domestica).


On supporting science journalism

If you're enjoying this article, consider supporting our award-winning journalism by subscribing. By purchasing a subscription you are helping to ensure the future of impactful stories about the discoveries and ideas shaping our world today.


Bristletails look like a cross between bite-sized shrimp and spiders. Their origins date back 390 million years to the early days of “insectdom”. Firebrats came 60 million years later; they resemble silverfish, those flat, metallic-shaded bugs that enjoy zipping around bathtubs and basements.

Previous research on insect olfaction has focused primarily on insects with wings, such as fruit flies (Drosophilamelanogaster). Bristletails and firebrats are wingless, but close relatives of the fruit fly, so Hansson and his team decided to draw an evolutionary picture of insect olfaction by comparing the three species.

Smelling in fruit flies starts with their antennae, which house smell sensory neurons coated with olfactory receptors (ORs) – protein “satellite receivers” that detect scent molecules. ORs are the present-day gear for smell. They are seen in most terrestrial mammals and insects with a sense of smell. ORs don’t work alone. Another receptor, called Orco, partners with ORs and tunes their response to odors. Orco, however, has never been shown to detect odors on its own.

The researchers scanned the bristletail genome and found no traces of ORs or Orco. Bristletail antennae, however, did respond to odors, albeit mildly relative to fruit flies. Bristletail antennae did accommodate a high density of ionotropic receptors. Thought of as a more ancient form of chemosensation, these proteins were recently implicated in the primitive smell system seen in dragonflies. Using electron microscopy, Hansson’s team looked at the architecture of sensilla, tiny cuticles on antennae that house sensory neurons. They found that the olfactory sensilla of bristletails, in general, lack the structure of modern insects.

Moving down the evolutionary road, firebrats were more bizarre. They had a stronger reaction to the same bouquet of aromas, but still lacked ORs. Firebrats did possess three copies of an Orco-related gene that were only produced by olfactory sensilla.

The results raise a bevy of questions such as: do firebrats represent an intermediate olfaction system where Orco plays a previously undefined role? The findings, however, argue that ORs and modern smell took time to develop.

“It can't have been the actual movement onto land itself that was the key change in their lifestyle,” says Richard Benton, a sensory evolution scientist at the University of Lusanne, Switzerland, who was not involved with the study.

Hansson theorizes that OR development was urged on by plants growing taller. As food resources moved upwards, insects developed wings and needed a way to find their meals. Supporting his hypothesis, his team examined and detected ORs and Orco in a fourth species, the leaf insect Phyllium siccifolium.

“Walking leafs represent a primitive form of modern, flying insects,” Hansson says. “We show that many tens of millions of years probably passed before the OR-Orco way of smelling developed. Indeed, we do not find it in any non-neopteran [non-flying] insect.”

Benton echoes this sentiment, though with a grain of temperance. “Additional species are needed to really reinforce this point, but it's a great start,” he says. “It should inspire more research into non-classic insect models, which will help fill the evolutionary picture of this sensory system.

Images courtesy of Bill Hansson.