They called him Diogenes the Cynic, because cynic meant dog-like, and he had a habit of basking naked on the lawn while his fellow philosophers talked on the porch. While they debated the mysteries of the cosmos, Diogenes preferred to soak up some rays - some have called him the Jimmy Buffett of ancient Greece.

Anyway, one morning, the great philosopher Plato had a stroke of insight. He caught everyones attention, gathered a crowd around him, and announced his deduction: Man is defined as a hairless, featherless, two-legged animal! Whereupon Diogenes abruptly leaped up from the lawn, dashed off to the marketplace, and burst back onto the porch carrying a plucked chicken - which he held aloft and shouted, Behold: I give you... Man!

Im sure Plato was less than thrilled at this stunt, but the story reminds us that these early philosophers were still hammering out the most basic tenets of the science we now know as taxonomy: The grouping of objects from the world into abstract categories. This technique of chopping up reality wasnt invented in ancient Greece, though. In fact, as a recent study shows, its fundamental to the way our brains work.

Chunks of reality

At the most basic level, we dont really perceive separate objects at all - we perceive our nervous systems responses to a boundless flow of electromagnetic waves and biochemical reactions. Our brains slot certain neural response patterns into sensory pathways we call sight, smell and so on - but abilities like synesthesia and echolocation show that even the boundaries between our senses can be blurry.

Semantic Space. Image: Gallant lab, UC Berkeley

Semantic Space. Image: Gallant lab, UC Berkeley

Still, our brains are talented at picking out certain chunks of sensory experience and associating those chunks with other stimuli. For instance, if you hear purring and feel fur rubbing against your leg, your brain knows to associate that sound and feeling with the fluffy four-legged object you see at your feet - and to group that whole multisensory chunk under the heading of cat.

Whats more, years of cat experience have taught you that it makes no sense to think of a cat as if it were a piece of furniture, or a truck, or a weather balloon. In other words, an encounter with a cat carries a particular set of meanings for you - and those meanings determine which areas of your brain will perk up in the presence of a feline.

But wheres the category cat in the brain? And wheres it situated in relation to, say, dog or giraffe ...or just mammal? A team of neuroscientists led by Alexander Huth at UC Berkeleys Gallant lab decided theyd answer these questions in the most thorough way possible: By capturing brain responses to every kind of object they could dig up.

Chunks in the brain

Those Gallant lab folks are no slouches - you might remember them as the lab that constructed mind videos of entire scenes from neural activity in the visual cortex. This time, though, the labs ambitions were even broader.

Semantic Map. Image: Gallant lab, UC Berkeley

Semantic Map. Image: Gallant lab, UC Berkeley

A research team led by Alex Huth showed volunteers hours of video footage of thousands of everyday objects and scenes - from cats and birds to cars and thunderstorms - as the subjects sat in an fMRI scanner. Then the researchers matched up the volunteers brain activity not only to each object they saw, but also to a whole tree of nested object categories: A taxonomy of the brains taxonomy. A vision of a continuous semantic space, where thousands of objects and actions are represented in terms of others.

Huths team collected volunteers reactions to more than 1,300 objects and categories, and arranged these brain responses not only into a tree of object and action categories, but into a map of response gradients across the whole surface of the brain.

And as you can see from the color gradients in that tree diagram to the right (which is also available as an interactive online app), the relationships among our brains categories are multidimensional. Objects may be more or less animal-like, more or less man-made, and so on - and in fact, the researchers say they expect to find more subtle response dimensions that gauge an objects size and speed.

Association and meaning

All this talk of dimensions of association points back to a far more profound idea about how our brains work: We understand the meaning of an object in terms of the meanings of other objects - other chunks of reality to which our brains have assigned certain characteristics. In the brains taxonomy, there are no discrete entries or files - just associations that are more strongly or more weakly correlated with other associations.

And that idea itself raises deeper quandaries: If associations define what an object or action is, as some neuroscientists have argued, then why does the concept of meaning - semantic representation - need to enter the picture at all? Instead of being a special type of mental function, might meaning itself simply be another word for association?

The answer to that question wont be a simple one to find, at least for the foreseeable future. I dont think its possible to make a conclusive claim about that from fMRI data, says Jack Gallant, the labs director; and anyone who tells you otherwise is mistaken.

A single three-dimensional pixel - an fMRI voxel - represents the activity of around one million neurons, Gallant explains; and at that resolution, its impossible to say what exactly the neural activity is encoding. Meaning could depend on association, association might depend on semantic coding, or the relationship between the two might be more nuanced than we can conceive right now.

Whatever that relationship turns out to be, the implication remains: In our brains, meaning and association go hand-in-hand. In the brain, even our most abstract concepts depend on our own real-world experiences. Thats an idea thats infuriated Plato and his followers far more than Diogenes plucked chicken - but as Diogenes demonstrated on that long-ago morning, real-world evidence trumps speculation in the end.